Giant rats could help detect illegal wildlife trade


Giant rats: A rat with a harness and a leash stands up by a box with its nose near the box.
A new study shows that African giant pouched rats can detect illegally trafficked wildlife, even when it has been concealed among other substances. Giant rats could be a new line of defense against the illegal wildlife trade. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.

The 2025 EarthSky Lunar Calendar presale is here! First 100 purchases signed by the legendary Deborah Byrd as a thank you. Get yours today!

  • African giant pouched rats have a keen sense of smell. They’ve been used in the past to sniff out explosives and more.
  • Now giant rats are learning to sniff out illegally trafficked wildlife, such as pangolin scales, elephant tusks, rhino horn and rare wood.
  • Smugglers who deal in illegal wildlife are also often involved in other illegal activities, including human, drug and arms trafficking. So the rats could assist with the global fight against networks that exploit humans and nature.

Frontiers published this original article on October 29, 2024. Edits by EarthSky.

Giant rats could help detect illegal wildlife trade

Pangolin scales, elephant tusks, rhino horn and a rare wood are preferred objects in the illegal wildlife trade. African giant pouched rats, with their keen sense of smell, however, could soon help to intercept this trade. Researchers showed the rats can successfully sniff out parts of endangered animals and remember the scent months after training. Employing them in the field could increase pressure on wildlife smugglers and add a flexibly deployable tool to screen cargo, the researchers said.

In the past, African giant pouched rats have learned to detect explosives and the tuberculosis-causing pathogen. Now, a team of researchers have trained these rats to pick up the scent of pangolin scales, elephant ivory, rhino horn and African blackwood. These animals and plants are threatened and at high danger of extinction.

Isabelle Szott, a researcher at the Okeanos Foundation, is the first co-author of the study published in Frontiers in Conservation Science. Szott said:

Our study shows that we can train African giant pouched rats to detect illegally trafficked wildlife, even when it has been concealed among other substances.

First co-author Kate Webb, an assistant professor at Duke University, added:

The rats also continued to detect the wildlife targets after not encountering that species for a long period.

The researchers conducted their study at APOPO, a Tanzania-based, non-profit organization that provides a low-tech, cost-efficient solutions to pressing humanitarian challenges.

Ratting out wildlife trafficking

The rats – Kirsty, Marty, Attenborough, Irwin, Betty, Teddy, Ivory, Ebony, Desmond, Thoreau and Fossey, some of them named after conservationists and advocates against wildlife trafficking – underwent several training stages. During indication training, the rats learned to hold their noses for several seconds in a hole where the target was. When they correctly performed this ‘nose poke,’ they received a reward with flavored rodent pellets.

In the next step, researchers introduced the rats to non-target odors. These included electric cables, coffee beans and washing powder. Smugglers frequently use these objects to mask the scent of wildlife in real-life trafficking operations. Szott said:

During the discrimination stage, rats learn to only signal the odors of the wildlife targets, while ignoring non-targets.

Researchers also trained the rats to remember smells. At the end of their retention training, they were reintroduced to scents they’d not encountered for five and eight months, respectively. Despite months of non-exposure, the rats showed perfect retention scores, suggesting that their cognitive retention performance resembles that of dogs.

By the end of the training, eight rats were able to identify four commonly smuggled wildlife species among 146 non-target substances.

Good giant rats!

A rat in a clear-walled cage with its nose in a hole in the floor.
During indication training, rats received rewards when they correctly performed a ‘nose poke’ in a hole containing a target. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A rat with its mouth on the syringe that a woman is holding.
During their training, the rats received rewards with flavored rodent pellets. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A rat in a clear-glass cage.
The researchers used a custom-built semi-automated line cage to train the rats. It has 10 holes along the floor, spaced 4 inches (10 cm) apart. Scent samples, in aluminum cassettes, were underneath the floor. Metal sliding plates in the floor can open and close for access to scent samples. Pictured here is the similar fully automated cage. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.

Rats in action

Szott explained:

Existing screening tools are expensive and time intensive, and there is an urgent need to increase cargo screening. APOPO’s rats are cost-efficient scent detection tools. They can easily access tight spaces like cargo in packed shipping containers or be lifted up high to screen the ventilation systems of sealed containers.

The next step, the scientists said, is to develop ways for the rats to work within ports through which smugglers trafficked wildlife. For this purpose, researchers will outfit the rats with custom-made vests. With their front paws, they will be able to pull a small ball attached at the chest of their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way rats will be able to alert their handlers when they detect a target. Webb said:

The vests are a great example of developing hardware that could be useful across different settings and tasks, including at a shipping port to detect smuggled wildlife.

This proof-of-principle study demonstrates that rats can successfully identify trafficked wildlife. This does not mean that it comes without limitations, the researchers said. For example, they conducted the study in a controlled environment, which is not reflective of the settings in which wildlife is commonly trafficked or screened by scent-detection animals. To deploy rats for this task, the researchers said they would need to develop new methods.

Webb concluded:

Wildlife smuggling is often conducted by individuals engaged in other illegal activities, including human, drug and arms trafficking. Therefore, deploying rats to combat wildlife trafficking may assist with the global fight against networks that exploit humans and nature.

Giant rats learning their roles

A rat with a red harness-type jacket holding something in its paws.
In real-life settings, rats will be able to pull a small ball attached at the chest of their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way rats will be able to alert their handlers when they detect a target. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A gloved hand holding a small white bowl with dark chips of something inside.
The rats learned to identify and remember the scent of 4 wildlife samples. These are samples of pangolin scales. All pangolin species are on the threatened tier of the IUCN Red List. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A bald black man holding a rat on his shoulder.
Giant African pouched rat with trainer. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.

Bottom line: Researchers are training giant rats to sniff out illegal wildlife samples in an effort to curb the trade of these materials.

Via Frontiers

Read more: Kangaroo rats are desert dwellers: Lifeform of the week

Read more: How naked mole rats stay cancer-free

The post Giant rats could help detect illegal wildlife trade first appeared on EarthSky.



from EarthSky https://ift.tt/YIxPn8i
Giant rats: A rat with a harness and a leash stands up by a box with its nose near the box.
A new study shows that African giant pouched rats can detect illegally trafficked wildlife, even when it has been concealed among other substances. Giant rats could be a new line of defense against the illegal wildlife trade. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.

The 2025 EarthSky Lunar Calendar presale is here! First 100 purchases signed by the legendary Deborah Byrd as a thank you. Get yours today!

  • African giant pouched rats have a keen sense of smell. They’ve been used in the past to sniff out explosives and more.
  • Now giant rats are learning to sniff out illegally trafficked wildlife, such as pangolin scales, elephant tusks, rhino horn and rare wood.
  • Smugglers who deal in illegal wildlife are also often involved in other illegal activities, including human, drug and arms trafficking. So the rats could assist with the global fight against networks that exploit humans and nature.

Frontiers published this original article on October 29, 2024. Edits by EarthSky.

Giant rats could help detect illegal wildlife trade

Pangolin scales, elephant tusks, rhino horn and a rare wood are preferred objects in the illegal wildlife trade. African giant pouched rats, with their keen sense of smell, however, could soon help to intercept this trade. Researchers showed the rats can successfully sniff out parts of endangered animals and remember the scent months after training. Employing them in the field could increase pressure on wildlife smugglers and add a flexibly deployable tool to screen cargo, the researchers said.

In the past, African giant pouched rats have learned to detect explosives and the tuberculosis-causing pathogen. Now, a team of researchers have trained these rats to pick up the scent of pangolin scales, elephant ivory, rhino horn and African blackwood. These animals and plants are threatened and at high danger of extinction.

Isabelle Szott, a researcher at the Okeanos Foundation, is the first co-author of the study published in Frontiers in Conservation Science. Szott said:

Our study shows that we can train African giant pouched rats to detect illegally trafficked wildlife, even when it has been concealed among other substances.

First co-author Kate Webb, an assistant professor at Duke University, added:

The rats also continued to detect the wildlife targets after not encountering that species for a long period.

The researchers conducted their study at APOPO, a Tanzania-based, non-profit organization that provides a low-tech, cost-efficient solutions to pressing humanitarian challenges.

Ratting out wildlife trafficking

The rats – Kirsty, Marty, Attenborough, Irwin, Betty, Teddy, Ivory, Ebony, Desmond, Thoreau and Fossey, some of them named after conservationists and advocates against wildlife trafficking – underwent several training stages. During indication training, the rats learned to hold their noses for several seconds in a hole where the target was. When they correctly performed this ‘nose poke,’ they received a reward with flavored rodent pellets.

In the next step, researchers introduced the rats to non-target odors. These included electric cables, coffee beans and washing powder. Smugglers frequently use these objects to mask the scent of wildlife in real-life trafficking operations. Szott said:

During the discrimination stage, rats learn to only signal the odors of the wildlife targets, while ignoring non-targets.

Researchers also trained the rats to remember smells. At the end of their retention training, they were reintroduced to scents they’d not encountered for five and eight months, respectively. Despite months of non-exposure, the rats showed perfect retention scores, suggesting that their cognitive retention performance resembles that of dogs.

By the end of the training, eight rats were able to identify four commonly smuggled wildlife species among 146 non-target substances.

Good giant rats!

A rat in a clear-walled cage with its nose in a hole in the floor.
During indication training, rats received rewards when they correctly performed a ‘nose poke’ in a hole containing a target. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A rat with its mouth on the syringe that a woman is holding.
During their training, the rats received rewards with flavored rodent pellets. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A rat in a clear-glass cage.
The researchers used a custom-built semi-automated line cage to train the rats. It has 10 holes along the floor, spaced 4 inches (10 cm) apart. Scent samples, in aluminum cassettes, were underneath the floor. Metal sliding plates in the floor can open and close for access to scent samples. Pictured here is the similar fully automated cage. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.

Rats in action

Szott explained:

Existing screening tools are expensive and time intensive, and there is an urgent need to increase cargo screening. APOPO’s rats are cost-efficient scent detection tools. They can easily access tight spaces like cargo in packed shipping containers or be lifted up high to screen the ventilation systems of sealed containers.

The next step, the scientists said, is to develop ways for the rats to work within ports through which smugglers trafficked wildlife. For this purpose, researchers will outfit the rats with custom-made vests. With their front paws, they will be able to pull a small ball attached at the chest of their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way rats will be able to alert their handlers when they detect a target. Webb said:

The vests are a great example of developing hardware that could be useful across different settings and tasks, including at a shipping port to detect smuggled wildlife.

This proof-of-principle study demonstrates that rats can successfully identify trafficked wildlife. This does not mean that it comes without limitations, the researchers said. For example, they conducted the study in a controlled environment, which is not reflective of the settings in which wildlife is commonly trafficked or screened by scent-detection animals. To deploy rats for this task, the researchers said they would need to develop new methods.

Webb concluded:

Wildlife smuggling is often conducted by individuals engaged in other illegal activities, including human, drug and arms trafficking. Therefore, deploying rats to combat wildlife trafficking may assist with the global fight against networks that exploit humans and nature.

Giant rats learning their roles

A rat with a red harness-type jacket holding something in its paws.
In real-life settings, rats will be able to pull a small ball attached at the chest of their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way rats will be able to alert their handlers when they detect a target. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A gloved hand holding a small white bowl with dark chips of something inside.
The rats learned to identify and remember the scent of 4 wildlife samples. These are samples of pangolin scales. All pangolin species are on the threatened tier of the IUCN Red List. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.
A bald black man holding a rat on his shoulder.
Giant African pouched rat with trainer. Image via APOPO/ Frontiers. Used with permission.

Bottom line: Researchers are training giant rats to sniff out illegal wildlife samples in an effort to curb the trade of these materials.

Via Frontiers

Read more: Kangaroo rats are desert dwellers: Lifeform of the week

Read more: How naked mole rats stay cancer-free

The post Giant rats could help detect illegal wildlife trade first appeared on EarthSky.



from EarthSky https://ift.tt/YIxPn8i

Aucun commentaire:

Enregistrer un commentaire